Mam language
This article has multiple issues. Please help improve it or discuss these issues on the talk page. (Learn how and when to remove these messages)
|
Mam | |
---|---|
Qyool Mam, Ta yol Mam | |
Native to | Guatemala, Mexico |
Region | Chiapas and Campeche, Mexico Quetzaltenango, Huehuetenango, San Marcos, and Retalhuleu, Guatemala; |
Ethnicity | Mam |
Native speakers | 600,000 in Guatemala (2019 census)[1] 10,000 in Mexico (2020 census)[2] |
Mayan
| |
Dialects |
|
Latin | |
Official status | |
Recognised minority language in | |
Regulated by | Instituto Nacional de Lenguas Indígenas Comunidad Lingüística Mam (COLIMAM) |
Language codes | |
ISO 639-3 | mam |
Glottolog | mamm1241 |
ELP | Mam |
Mam is a Mayan language spoken by about half a million Mam people in the Guatemalan departments of Quetzaltenango, Huehuetenango, San Marcos, and Retalhuleu, and the Mexican states of Campeche and Chiapas. Thousands more make up a Mam diaspora throughout the United States and Mexico, with notable populations living in Oakland, California[3][4] and Washington, D.C. The most extensive Mam grammar is Nora C. England's A grammar of Mam, a Mayan language (1983), which is based on the San Ildefonso Ixtahuacán dialect of Huehuetenango Department.
Classification
Mam is closely related to the Tektitek language, and the two languages together form the Mamean sub-branch of the Mayan language family. Along with the Ixilan languages, Awakatek and Ixil, these make up the Greater Mamean sub-branch, one of the two branches of the Eastern Mayan languages (the other being the Greater Quichean sub-branch, which consists of 10 Mayan languages, including Kʼicheʼ).
Dialects
Because contact between members of different Mam communities is somewhat limited, the language varies considerably even from village to village. Nevertheless, mutual intelligibility, though difficult, is possible through practice.[5]
Mam varieties within Mexico and Guatemala are divided into five dialect groups:[6]
- Northern Mam in Campeche, Mexico and southern Huehuetenango Department, Guatemala.[7] Northern Mam is the least conservative group according to Terrence Kaufman.[5]
- Southern Mam in southern Campeche, Mexico and Quetzaltenango Department, San Marcos Department, and Retalhuleu Department, Guatemala.[8]
- Central Mam in Chiapas, Mexico and San Marcos Department, Guatemala.
- Western Mam in eastern Chiapas, Mexico and northwestern San Marcos Department, Guatemala.[9] The Tektitek language may be mutually intelligible with Western Mam dialects.
- Soconusco Mam in the Soconusco region, Chiapas, Mexico[10]
In addition to these, the dialects of Chiapas, Mexico are characterized by significant grammatical as well as lexical differences from the Guatemalan varieties.[11]
Distribution
Mam is spoken in 64 communities in four Guatemalan departments[12] and numerous communities in Campeche and Chiapas, Mexico.[11] Neighboring languages include Jakaltek and Qʼanjobʼal to the north, Tektitek and Qato'k to the west, and Ixil, Awakatek, Sipacapense, and Kʼicheʼ to the east.
Phonology
Stress
Mam has weight sensitive stress assignment.[13] Primary stress falls on the long vowel in a word if there is one, e.g. aq'ú:ntl 'work'. Words without a long vowel assign primary stress to the vowel preceding the last glottal stop, e.g. puʔláʔ 'dipper'. Words without a long vowel or a glottal stop assign stress to the vowel preceding the last consonant in the root, e.g. xpicháqʼ 'raccoon'. Stress is not assigned to suffixes or enclitics that do not have long vowels or a glottal stop.
Vowels
Mam has 10 vowels, 5 short and 5 long:[14]
Front | Central | Back | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
Close | Long | iː ⟨ii⟩ | uː ⟨uu⟩ | |
Short | ɪ ⟨i⟩ | ʊ ⟨u⟩ | ||
Mid | Long | eː ⟨ee⟩ | oː ⟨oo⟩ | |
Short | ɛ ⟨e⟩ | ɔ ⟨o⟩ | ||
Open | Long | aː ⟨aa⟩ | ||
Short | a ⟨a⟩ |
- A mid-central vowel [ə] is an allophone of a short unstressed vowel that can occur in the syllable following a stressed long vowel.
Consonants
Mam has 27 consonants, including the glottal stop:[5]
Bilabial | Alveolar | Postalveolar | Retroflex | Palatal | Velar | Uvular | Glottal | |||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Plain | Palatalized | |||||||||
Nasal | m ⟨m⟩ | n ⟨n⟩ | (ŋ ⟨n⟩) | |||||||
Plosive | Plain | p ⟨p⟩ | t ⟨t⟩ | k ⟨k⟩ | kʲ ⟨ky⟩ | q ⟨q⟩ | ʔ ⟨ʼ⟩ | |||
Ejective | tʼ~ɗ̥ ⟨tʼ⟩ | kʼ ⟨kʼ⟩ | kʲʼ ⟨kyʼ⟩ | |||||||
Implosive | ɓ~ɓ̥ ⟨bʼ⟩ | ʛ̥ ⟨qʼ⟩ | ||||||||
Affricate | Plain | t͡s ⟨tz⟩ | t͡ʃ ⟨ch⟩ | ʈ͡ʂ ⟨tx⟩ | ||||||
Ejective | t͡sʼ ⟨tzʼ⟩ | t͡ʃʼ ⟨chʼ⟩ | ʈ͡ʂʼ ⟨txʼ⟩ | |||||||
Fricative | s ⟨s⟩ | ʃ ⟨ẍ (xh)⟩ | ʂ ⟨x⟩ | χ ⟨j⟩ | ||||||
Flap | ɾ ⟨r⟩ | |||||||||
Approximant | l ⟨l⟩ | j ⟨y⟩ | w ⟨w⟩ |
- Stop sounds /p, t, t͡s, t͡ʃ, t͡ʂ, k, q/ are released with aspiration [Cʰ] in word-final position.
- Todos Santos Mam has an extended amount of affricate consonants being apical palato-alveolar /t̺͡ʃ̺ʰ, t̺͡ʃ̺ʼ, ʃ̺/.[15]
Syllable structure
Most roots take the morphological shape CVC.[16] The only possible root final consonant cluster is -nC. Syllables can have up to four consonants in a cluster in any position. Most consonant clusters are the result of vowel dropping and morpheme addition.[17]
Morphology
Mam has two sets of agreement markers, known to Mayanists as Set A and Set B markers, which can appear on both nouns and verbs. Mam uses Set A (ergative) markers on nouns to mark possessor agreement and on verbs to cross-reference the transitive subject. Mam uses Set B (absolutive) markers on transitive verbs to cross-reference the object and on intransitive verbs to cross-reference the subject. Below is a table of Set A (ergative) and Set B (absolutive) prefixes from England.[18]
Person | Set A | Set B | Enclitics |
---|---|---|---|
1s | n- ~ w- | chin- | -a ~ -ya |
2s | t- | Ø ~ tz- ~ tzʼ- ~ k- | -a ~ -ya |
3s | t- | Ø ~ tz- ~ tzʼ- ~ k- | – |
1p (excl.) | q- | qo- | -a ~ -ya |
1p (incl.) | q- | qo- | – |
2p | ky- | chi- | -a ~ -ya |
3p | ky- | chi- | – |
Phonologically conditioned allomorphs are as follows.
- n- ~ w-
- n- /__C
- w- /__V
- Ø ~ tz- ~ tzʼ- ~ k-
- k- /potential
- tzʼ- /__V initial root, non-potential
- tz- /__uul 'arrive here', iky' 'pass by', non-potential
- Ø- /__C, non-potential
- -a ~ -ya
- -ya /V__ ; In the first person in post-vowel environments, -ya varies freely with -kyʼa and -y'.
- -a /C__
Some paradigmatic examples from England (1983) are given below. Note that "Ø-" designates a null prefix. Additionally, ma is an aspectual word meaning 'recent past'.
Set A markers + NOUN | |
---|---|
jaa | 'house' |
n-jaa-ya | 'my house' |
t-jaa-ya | 'your house' |
t-jaa | 'his/her house' |
q-jaa-ya | 'our (not your) house' |
q-jaa | 'our (everyone's) house' |
ky-jaa-ya | 'you (pl)'s house' |
ky-jaa | 'their house' |
Set B markers + VERB | |
---|---|
bʼeet- | to walk |
ma chin bʼeet-a | 'I walked.' |
ma Ø-bʼeet-a | 'You walked.' |
ma Ø-bʼeet | 'He/she walked.' |
ma qo bʼeet-a | 'We (not you) walked.' |
ma qo bʼeet | 'We all walked' |
ma chi bʼeet-a | 'You all walked.' |
ma chi bʼeet | 'They walked.' |
The Mam verb complex
Verbs in Mam can include inflection for person, aspect and mode, as well as auxiliaries in the form of directionals.[19] The verb complex has distinct forms for transitive and intransitive verb stems depending in part on whether the complex cross-references one or two arguments. The lexical status of the verb complex is ambiguous.[20] The inflections with vowels are phonologically independent (indicated by spaces). Transitive verb complexes with directionals have a dependent suffix. Two of England's examples of intransitive and transitive verb complexes are shown below.
Intransitive verb complex with directional[21]
Transitive verb complex with directional[22]
Mam extends the Set A (ergative) person markers in the context of focused adverbials and certain subordinate clauses.[23] In these contexts, the Set A markers cross-reference the subject of intransitive verbs and both the subject and object of transitive verbs. The following examples show the extended ergative marker /t-/ in bold.
Intransitive verb complex with extended ergative marking[23]
ooq'
cry
"They were crying when their mother arrived."
Transitive verb complex with extended ergative marking[23]
ok
When
xjaal
person
kjoʔn
cornfield
b'iʔx
all_at_once
cheenaq'
bean
"When the people plant (it) in the cornfield at the same time the beans go in."
Verb morphemes
Transitive verbal affixes
Intransitive verbal affixes
|
Other verbal affixes
Aspects Mam verbs have 6 aspects that are prefixed to the verb root.[24]
Modes
Directionals Directionals are auxiliary elements in verb phrases. They are derived from intransitive verbs.
|
Pronouns
Mam has no independent pronouns.[25] Rather, pronouns in Mam always exist as bound morphemes.
Nouns
The Mam language displays inalienable possession. Certain Mam nouns cannot be possessed, such as kya'j 'sky' and che'w 'star'.[26] On the other hand, some Mam nouns are always possessed, such as t-lokʼ 'its root' and t-bʼaqʼ 'its seed'.
Noun phrase structure can be summarized into the following template.[27]
Demonstrative Number Measure Plural Possessive affixes NOUN
ROOTPossessor Adjective Relative clause
The plural clitic is qa.[citation needed]
Noun affixes
Relational noun affixes
Locative affixes
|
Classifiers
Measure words Measure words quantify mass nouns.
|
Numerals
San Ildefonso Ixtahuacán Mam numbers are as follows.[28] Numbers above twenty are rarely used in Ixtahuacán and are usually only known by elderly speakers. Although the number system would have originally been vigesimal (i.e., base 20), the present-day number system of Ixtahuacán is now decimal.
Numeral | Word |
---|---|
1 | juun |
2 | kabʼ |
3 | oox |
4 | kyaaj |
5 | jweʼ |
6 | qaq |
7 | wuuq |
8 | wajxaq |
9 | bʼelaj |
10 | laaj |
20 | wiinqan |
40 | kyaʼwnaq |
60 | oxkʼaal |
80 | junmutxʼ |
Syntax
Mam has both verbal and non-verbal types of sentences. Verbal sentences have verbal predicates, whereas non-verbal sentences have a stative or a locative/existential predicate.[29] Verbal predicates have an aspect marker, while non-verbal predicates do not have aspect marking. Both verbal and non-verbal predicates occur in sentence-initial position unless a focused or topicalized phrase is present.
Verbal predicates
Verbal predicates are either transitive or intransitive according to the number of arguments cross-referenced in the verb complex. The number of arguments cross-referenced by the verb complex is not consistent with the transitivity of the verb root or the number of participants in an event. England notes examples of transitive verb roots that only appear in their antipassive or passive forms where they only cross-reference a single participant.
- Transitive verb root with obligatory antipassive voice[30]
ma-yax
REC-INTENS
xuʔj
woman
nim-b'ee
big-road
"The woman laughed a lot in the road."
- Transitive verb root with obligatory passive voice[31]
ma
REC
jun
one
tz'iis
garbage
"I found my penny in the garbage." (Lit. "My penny was found in the garbage")
Another possibility is the use of intransitive motion verbs to express transitive events.[32]
- Intransitive motion verbs expressing transitive events[32]
aʔ
water
"I will bring water." (Lit. "Water will come by me.")
o
ich'
mouse
"His cat killed mice." (Lit. "His cat went down at mice.")
The basic word order in verbal sentences with two nominal arguments is VSO.[33] Other word orders are not acceptable.
If only one argument appears in a transitive sentence and the argument is compatible with either person marker on the verb, it has a patient interpretation.[34]
ma
REC
xiinaq
man
"They grabbed the man." (Not "The men grabbed it.")
Mam speakers use a higher proportion of intransitive sentences than speakers of other Mayan languages. England and Martin (2003) found a low frequency of transitive sentences in Mam texts.[full citation needed] Pye (2017) found a low use of overt subjects in transitive sentences in adults speaking to children. One adult produced overt subjects in 6% of transitive sentences. The same adult produced overt subjects in 41% of intransitive sentences and produced overt objects in 49% of transitive sentences.[35]
Non-verbal predicates
Mam adds Set B person markers to nouns and adjectives to form non-verbal predicates. The following Set B person markers are used for non-verbal predicates (i.e., nouns, adjectives). Also, in statives, aa can be omitted when the rest of the stative is a non-enclitic (in other words, a separate, independent word).
Person | Stative[36] | Locative / Existental[37] |
---|---|---|
1s | (aa) qiin-a | (a)t-iin-a |
2s | aa-ya | (a)t-(aʼ-y)a |
3s | aa | (a)t-(aʼ) |
1p (excl.) | (aa) qoʼ-ya | (a)t-oʼ-ya |
1p (incl.) | (aa) qoʼ | (a)t-oʼ |
2p | aa-qa-ya | (a)t-eʼ-ya |
3p | aa-qa | (a)t-eʼ |
Paradigmatic examples of non-verbal predicates from England (1983) are given below.[38]
NOUN + Set B markers | |
---|---|
xjaal | person |
xjaal qiin-a | 'I am a person.' |
xjaal-a | 'You are a person.' |
xjaal | 'He/she is a person.' |
xjaal qoʼ-ya | 'We (excl.) are persons.' |
xjaal qo- | 'We (incl.) are persons.' |
xjaal qa-ya | 'You all are persons.' |
xjaal qa | 'They are persons.' |
ADJECTIVE + Set B markers | |
---|---|
sikynaj | tired |
sikynaj qiin-a | 'I am tired.' |
sikynaj-a | 'You are tired.' |
sikynaj | 'He/she is tired.' |
sikynaj qoʼ-ya | 'We (excl.) are tired.' |
sikynaj qoʼ | 'We (incl.) are tired.' |
sikynaj qa-ya | 'You all are tired.' |
sikynaj qa | 'They are tired.' |
Child Language
An overview of child language acquisition in Mam can be found in Pye (2017). Child language data for Mam challenge many theories of language acquisition and demonstrate the need for more extensive documentation of native American languages.[39]
Children acquiring Mam produce a higher proportion of verbs than children acquiring K’iche’, but a lower proportion of verbs compared to children acquiring Wastek and Chol. They produce a higher proportion of intransitive verbs relative to transitive verbs than children acquiring other Mayan languages (Pye, Pfeiler and Mateo Pedro 2017:22).[clarification needed] Their high proportion of relational noun production is tied to their frequent use of intransitive verbs.[40]
The following examples illustrate the children's use of intransitive verbs to express events with two participants. Ages are shown as (years;months.days). WEN (2;0.2) used the intransitive verb -kub’ ("go_down") in reference to an event of picking coffee. She used the relational noun phrase t-uʔn-a to express the agent in an oblique phrase. CRU (2;5.12) used the intransitive verb -el ("go_out") in reference to an event of taking out an object. She used the relational noun phrase w-uʔn-a to express the agent. JOS (2;6.17) used the intransitive verb -b’aj ("finish") in reference to finishing a drink. He used the possessive prefix on the noun k’aʔ ("drink")to express the agent. The examples overturn the hypothesis that children tie their use of transitive verbs to object manipulation events.
- WEN (2;0.2)
ma
REC
pa
already
pe
kape
coffee
"Did you already pick the coffee?" (Lit. "Did the coffee already go down by you?")
- CRU (2;5.12)
"I can’t get it to come out." (Lit. "It is not coming out by me.")
- JOS (2;6.17)
kal
tqal
what
"What is he drinking?" (Lit. "What is his drink that is finishing?")
Two-year-old Mam children produce the consonants /m, n, p, t, ch, k, ʔ, l, y and w/. They produce [ʔ] in place of glottalized stops, [p] in place of /ɓ/, [k] in place of /ky/[clarification needed] and /q/, /ch/[clarification needed] in place of /tz/[clarification needed] and /tx/[clarification needed], /xh/[clarification needed] in place of /x/[clarification needed], and [l] in place of /r/. Mam children begin producing ejective consonants after they are three and a half years old.[41] The early production of /ch/ and /l/ in Mam, as well as the late production of /s/, overturns predictions that all children have similar phonologies due to articulatory development.[citation needed]
The acquisition of morphology in Mam is heavily influenced by prosody. Two-year-old children favor the production of word syllables with primary stress, and most often produce syllables with the form CVC. Children do not consistently produce inflectional prefixes on nouns and verbs before they are four years old, although two-year-olds frequently produce verb suffixes, including the directional suffixes. Their production of the directional suffixes is evidence that two-year-old Mam children understand the complex grammatical constraints on the use of directionals. They distinguish between the use of the directional clitics and directional suffixes in indicative and imperative verbs. Two-year-old Mam speakers omit the person enclitic on nouns and verbs despite its high frequency of use in adult speech.[citation needed]
The following examples illustrate WEN’s verb complex production.[42] In (1), WEN produced the vowel /a/ from the verb root -q'a ("give"), the imperative suffix -n, and the directional suffix -tz as /xh/. (Many directionals have contracted forms as suffixes.). WEN omitted the person enclitic -a. In (2), WEN produced the progressive prefix n-, the vowel /e/ from the verb root -el ("go out"), a spurious /n/, and the directional suffix -tz as /ch/. The intransitive verb -el belongs to the class of motion verbs that take directional suffixes. Intransitive verbs outside of the class of motion verbs do not take directional suffixes except in imperative contexts. The verb -el contracts with the directional suffix -tz to produce the stem -etz ("go out to") in adult speech. WEN’s omission of the person enclitic and production of a spurious consonant overturn the hypothesis that children produce forms that are frequent in adult speech.
- WEN (1;9.2)
- WEN (1;8.21)
The children’s production of the directional suffixes demonstrates their early recognition of the distinction between intransitive and transitive verbs in Mam. This distinction is a core feature of Mam grammar, and underpins the ergative morphology on the verbs and nouns. The semantic diversity of the verbs and positionals overturns the hypothesis that children use prototypical activity scenes as a basis for constructing grammatical categories. The children’s grammatical acumen is best seen in their use of the ergative and absolutive agreement markers on verbs. The children produced the prevocalic allomorphs of the ergative markers in nearly all of their obligatory contexts. They produced the preconsonantal allomorphs of the ergative markers in 20% of their obligatory contexts.[40]
Two-year-old Mam children display a remarkable awareness of the contexts for extending the use of ergative markers to cross-reference the subject of intransitive verbs. Outside of these contexts, they consistently produced absolutive person markers on intransitive verbs. Three Mam children produced ergative person markers on intransitive verbs in half of the obligatory contexts for extended ergativity.[43] The children’s awareness of the contexts for extended ergative use is all the more remarkable because the contexts are tied to clauses in dependent contexts in which aspect is not overtly marked.[44] The following example shows JOS’s use of extended ergative marking (in bold) on the intransitive verb -ok ("go_in") in a purpose clause headed by the adverb ii ("so that"). The children’s production of ergative markers on intransitive verbs in dependent contexts overturns the theory that children link ergative markers to the subjects of transitive verbs in all contexts.
- JOS (2;6.14)
Mam two-year-olds produce sentences with a predicate-initial word order. The children, like adults, rarely produce the subject argument in transitive sentences. The Mam children show an ergative pattern of argument production that similar to the adult pattern.[45] The children produced subject arguments in 7 percent or fewer of sentences with transitive verbs. The children produced subject arguments in 40 percent of sentences with intransitive verbs, and produced object arguments in 45 percent of sentences with transitive verbs.[citation needed]
The acquisition data for Mam and other Mayan languages have profound implications for language acquisition theory. Children demonstrate an early proficiency with verb inflection in languages with a rich morphology and where the language’s prosodic structure highlights the morphology. The Mam children’s use of directionals and extended ergative marking shows that two-year-olds are capable of using complex affixes appropriately in their obligatory contexts. This morphology accounts for the language-specific look of the children’s early utterances and guides its development in later stages.[citation needed]
References
Notes
- ^ Mam at Ethnologue (24th ed., 2021)
- ^ Lenguas indígenas y hablantes de 3 años y más, 2020 INEGI. Censo de Población y Vivienda 2020.
- ^ Carcamo, Cindy (9 August 2016). "Ancient Mayan languages are creating problems for today's immigration courts". Los Angeles Times. Retrieved 2018-11-24.
- ^ Farida Jhabvala Romero (August 19, 2019). "Growth of Oakland's Guatemalan community sparks interest in Mam". PRI's The World. PRI. Retrieved August 22, 2019.
- ^ a b c England 1983.
- ^ Pérez Vail, Eduardo Gustavo (2004). Gramática Pedagógica Mam. Guatemala: Instituto de Lingüística y Educación, Universidad Rafael Landívar.
- ^ "Catálogo de las lenguas indígenas nacionales: Mam del norte".
- ^ "Catálogo de las lenguas indígenas nacionales: Mam del sur".
- ^ "Catálogo de las lenguas indígenas nacionales: Mam de la frontera".
- ^ "Catálogo de las lenguas indígenas nacionales: Mam del Soconusco".
- ^ a b Ramos Ortíz, Nicacio; Morales de León, Juan Rolando; Rodriguez Pérez, Juan (2013). Gramática Didáctica Mam: Segundo Ciclo. Mexico: Instituto Nacional de Lenguas Indígenas.
- ^ Academia de Lenguas Mayas de Guatemala (2003). Pujbʼil Yol Mam: Vocabulario Mam.
- ^ England 1983, pp. 37–38.
- ^ England 1983, p. 33.
- ^ England, Nora C. (2017). Mam. The Mayan Languages: London & New York: Routledge. pp. 500–532.
- ^ England 1983, p. 93.
- ^ England 1983, p. 38.
- ^ England 1983, p. 56.
- ^ England 1983, p. 161.
- ^ England 1983, p. 40.
- ^ England 1983, p. 162.
- ^ England 1983, p. 175.
- ^ a b c England 1983, p. 259.
- ^ England 1983, pp. 58, 161–162.
- ^ England 1983, p. 155.
- ^ England 1983, p. 69.
- ^ England 1983, p. 140.
- ^ England 1983, p. 84.
- ^ England 1983, p. 177.
- ^ England 1983, p. 178.
- ^ England 1983, p. 180.
- ^ a b England 1983, p. 181.
- ^ England 1983, p. 193.
- ^ England 1983, p. 194.
- ^ Pye 2017, pp. 114–115.
- ^ Means 'This is X.'
- ^ Means 'X is in a place.'
- ^ England 1983, p. 76.
- ^ Pye, Clifton (May 2021). "Documenting the acquisition of indigenous languages". Journal of Child Language. 48 (3): 454–479. doi:10.1017/S0305000920000318. ISSN 0305-0009. PMID 32500845.
- ^ a b Pye 2017.
- ^ Pye, Clifton; Mateo, Pedro; Pfeiler, Barbara; Stengel, Donald (2017-10-01). "Analysis of variation in Mayan child phonologies". Lingua. 198: 38–52. doi:10.1016/j.lingua.2017.07.001. ISSN 0024-3841.
- ^ Pye & Pfeiler 2019.
- ^ Pye, Pfeiler & Mateo Pedro 2013, p. 323.
- ^ England 1983, p. 264.
- ^ Pye 2017, pp. 224–225.
Bibliography
- Pérez, Eduardo; Jiménez, Odilio (1997). Ttxoolil Qyool Mam – Gramática Mam. Cholsamaj.
- England, Nora C. (1983). A grammar of Mam, a Mayan language. Austin: University of Texas Press. ISBN 978-0-292-72927-8.
- England, Nora C.; Martin, Laura (2003). "Issues in the comparative argument structure analysis in Mayan narratives". In Du Bois, J. W.; Kumpf, L. E.; Ashby, W. J. (eds.). Preferred Argument Structures. John Benjamins. pp. 130–155.
- Pye, Clifton (2017). The Comparative Method of Language Acquisition Research. The University of Chicago Press. ISBN 9780226481289.
- Pye, Clifton (2021). "Documenting the acquisition of indigenous languages". Journal of Child Language. 48 (3): 454–479. doi:10.1017/S0305000920000318. PMID 32500845. S2CID 219327130.
- Pye, Clifton; Pfeiler, Barbara; Mateo Pedro, Pedro (2013). "The acquisition of extended ergativity in Mam, Q'anjob'al and Yucatec". In Bavin, Edith L.; Still, Sabine (eds.). The Acquisition of Ergativity. John Benjamins. pp. 307–335.
- Pye, Clifton; Mateo, Pedro; Pfeiler, Barbara; Stengel, Donald (2017). "Analysis of variation in Mayan child phonologies". Lingua. 198: 38–52. doi:10.1016/j.lingua.2017.07.001.
- Pye, Clifton; Pfeiler, Barbara (2019). "The acquisition of directionals in two Mayan languages". Front. Psychol. 10: 2442. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02442. PMC 6839415. PMID 31736835.
Further reading
- Bʼaayil; Pérez, Eduardo (2000). Variación dialectal en mam = Txʼixpubʼente tiibʼ qyool / Proyecto de Investigación Lingüística de Oxlajuuj Keej Mayaʼ Ajtzʼiibʼ. Guatemala, Guatemala: Cholsamaj. ISBN 9789992253113.
- Pujbʼil yol mam / Kʼulbʼil Yol Twitz Paxil; Kʼulbʼil Yol Mam = Vocabulario mam / Academia de Lenguas Mayas de Guatemala; Comunidad Lingüística Mam. Guatemala, Guatemala: Kʼulbʼil Yol Twitz Paxil. 2003.
- Rojas Ramírez, Maximiliano (1993). Gramática del idioma Mam. La Antigua Guatemala, Guatemala: Proyecto Lingüístico Francisco Marroquín.
External links
- A simple Mam – Spanish dictionary
- Robert Sitler's Mam – English dictionary (DOC) (PDF)
- Key phrases in Mam vs Spanish
- Books from Cholsamaj
- Mayan Languages Collection of Nora England at the Archive of the Indigenous Languages of Latin America, containing recordings and transcriptions of narratives and conversations in Mam.