Jump to content

Counterstereotype

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
The printable version is no longer supported and may have rendering errors. Please update your browser bookmarks and please use the default browser print function instead.

A counterstereotype is an idea or object that goes against a stereotype -- a standardized mental picture that is held in common by members of a group and that represents an oversimplified opinion, prejudiced attitude, or uncritical judgment.[1]

Process

Background

Minority groups within society are often portrayed negatively within popular media. Through psychological processes such as priming, this reinforces consumers’ negative stereotypes toward those groups[2][3][4]. Bombarding consumers with these stereotypes causes implicit attitudes - which occurs in the absence of conscious thought - to be negative towards those groups. This has a wide range of consequences, with the most profound effects being seen in exposure to the “negative” group in ambiguous situations[3]. An example of this can be seen with the over-portrayal of African-Americans as criminals in American media: the psychological literature shows that through media reinforcement of a criminal stereotype, consumers of this content evaluate African-Americans as more dangerous than other groups even in ambiguous situations.[3] This demonstrates how media depiction of stereotypes can lead directly to prejudice. This is consequential because it can affect not only inter-group interactions, but the self-identity of the “negative group” (see: Self-fulfilling prophecy). Psychological research has shown that the stereotype associated simply with one’s name can cause changes in behavior and cognition[5].

Counter-stereotypes work by presenting media consumers with a member of a “negative” group who does not act stereotypically. This challenges consumers’ existing attitudes towards that group, and has been shown to reduce existing prejudices toward them. Exposing consumers to counter-stereotypes may also lead to increased support for policy options aimed at addressing racial disparities, such as affirmative action[3].

Exemplar Models

Exemplar-based explanations of counter-stereotyping have gained much support from the psychological literature[3]. Prototypes can best be described as ideas of what a category is, formed by averaging all instances of that category that an individual has come into contact with. Exemplars are specific representations of that “averaged” category, and can serve to change the prototype[3]. By presenting consumers with prototypes not consistent with their existing representation of that group, counter stereotypes can change implicit attitudes toward the target group. The recency and frequency of exposure to the exemplar mediate how stereotypes change over time, with those that are chronically accessible being the most effective of all.[3]

Categorization-Processing-Adaptation-Generalization Model

The Categorization Processing Adaptation Generalization model builds upon previous cognitive research as to how diversity experiences, or in essence counterstereotypical experiences can change the way people think.[6][7] When encountering counter stereotypical events, if a perceiver is motivated to engage in elaborative processing involving stereotype suppression and generative thought, that with multiple experiences resulting in elaborative processing, the perceiver will develop generalized cognitive flexibility.[6] This would mean the use of elaborative thought over heuristics based thinking, and weakens reliance on quick mental representations such as stereotypes.

Limitations

Selection Bias

One major limitation of the Counterstereotype effect is that it may not reach the audience which would benefit most from exposure because that audience chooses not to view content with minority characters. In the last few decades, the ability for audiences to be selective with their choice for media consumption has increased rapidly and led to the rise of the echo chamber. Individuals who hold an explicit negative stereotype of a certain group have the option to avoid all content which contains that group, preventing exposure to the counter-stereotype altogether.[2] Research into this phenomenon also suggests that negative stereotypes don’t have to be held explicitly to affect media viewing habits: implicit attitudes, which are not held consciously, affect the willingness of individuals to consume content depicting certain groups[3]. In short, the counter-stereotype effect is limited not by the process in which it occurs, but by its limited ability to reach individuals who already hold negative attitudes towards the target group.

Backlash Effects

Not to be confused with the boomerang effect which refers to unintended psychosocial consequences resulting from persuasion, nor backlash effects in sociology, the backlash effect is similar in principle, having to do with psychosocial consequences resulting from perception of being counter stereotypical. Most cited definition of the backlash effect by Rudman in 1998[8] is where violation of stereotypes may potentially lead to social or economic consequences. Although these counterstereotypical people may be perceived as unique, the backlash effect and its consequences often limit their success, and can lead to a reinforcement of stereotypes.

A 2004 study by Rudman and Fairchild[9] modeled the backlash effect and stereotype maintenance stemming from perceivers and actors, their justification or fear of backlash, and the importance in the maintenance of self-esteem. From the perceivers view, if it is justified by a threat to their self-esteem, backlash towards counter stereotypical people may restore their sense of worth, however this only further reinforces societal stereotypes. The backlash effect can also lead counter stereotypical individuals to fear the potential consequences and thus be more likely to hide their counter stereotypical behavior, maintaining their self-esteem. This also leads to the maintenance of cultural stereotypes.

Examples of backlash effects
  • Women being underrepresented in high level roles in some industries resulting from withholding promotions to women who display counter stereotypical behaviour, or women not wanting to exhibit counter stereotypical behaviour as a fear of backlash. [10]
  • The marvel character Miles Morales whom is a version of Spiderman that is often the white Peter Parker as had several waves of backlash directed at the character and creators [11][12]
  • Minority actors in Hollywood despite showing great talent, being typecast in cheap caricature roles, and backlash towards their success in the industry as a whole [13][14]

Notable Examples

  • An example of a counter-stereotype is the Fifteenth Doctor, a character in the British TV series Dr. Who. Previously played exclusively by white actors, the inclusion of a black actor for this role served to combat existing stereotypes in British society.[15]
  • In the film Slumdog Millionaire, the main character displays exceptional intellectual abilities and wins a large monetary prize. This challenges the stereotype that those living in poverty are not intelligent.
  • American popular literature in the 19th century contained stereotypical images of black people as grotesque and servile. In protest, a counter-stereotype arose which showed black people as graceful and wise.[16]
  • In the United States during the 1970s, in response to feminist criticism, advertising agencies chose to display counter-stereotypical images of women as sexually assertive and intellectual.[17]
  • In the film Legally Blonde, the main character, Elle Woods, acts as a counter-stereotype displaying how a blonde, conventionally attractive and fashion-loving woman is also intelligent and successful at attending law school.[18]


See also

References

  1. ^ "Definition of STEREOTYPE". www.merriam-webster.com. 2024-10-18. Retrieved 2024-10-26.
  2. ^ a b Mustafaj, Matea; Dal Cin, Sonya (2023). "Preexisting Stereotypes and Selection of Counter-Stereotypical Genius Representations in Entertainment Media". Journal of Media Psychology. 36 (1): 1–14. doi:10.1027/1864-1105/a000377. ISSN 1864-1105.
  3. ^ a b c d e f g h Ramasubramanian, Srividya (2011-01-10). "The Impact of Stereotypical Versus Counterstereotypical Media Exemplars on Racial Attitudes, Causal Attributions, and Support for Affirmative Action". Communication Research. 38 (4): 497–516. doi:10.1177/0093650210384854. ISSN 0093-6502.
  4. ^ Dixon, Travis L.; Azocar, Cristina L (2006-03-10). "Priming Crime and Activating Blackness: Understanding the Psychological Impact of the Overrepresentation of Blacks as Lawbreakers on Television News". Journal of Communication. 57 (2): 230. doi:10.1111/j.1460-2466.2007.00341.x. ISSN 0021-9916.
  5. ^ Eichler, Ernst; Hilty, Gerold; Löffler, Heinrich; Steger, Hugo, eds. (1996-01-11), "Personal Name Stereotypes Personennamen-Stereotypen Stéréotypes des noms de personnes", Namenforschung / Name Studies / Les noms propres, Part 2, Berlin • New York: Walter de Gruyter, doi:10.1515/9783110148794.2.21.1744, ISBN 978-3-11-020343-1, retrieved 2024-11-29
  6. ^ a b Crisp, Richard J.; Turner, Rhiannon N. (March 2011). "Cognitive adaptation to the experience of social and cultural diversity". Psychological Bulletin. 137 (2): 242–266. doi:10.1037/a0021840. ISSN 1939-1455.
  7. ^ Fiske, Susan T.; Neuberg, Steven L. (1990), "A Continuum of Impression Formation, from Category-Based to Individuating Processes: Influences of Information and Motivation on Attention and Interpretation", Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, Elsevier, pp. 1–74, retrieved 2024-11-30
  8. ^ Rudman, Laurie A. (1998). "Self-promotion as a risk factor for women: The costs and benefits of counterstereotypical impression management". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 74 (3): 629–645. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.3.629. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 9523410.
  9. ^ Rudman, Laurie A.; Fairchild, Kimberly (2004). "Reactions to Counterstereotypic Behavior: The Role of Backlash in Cultural Stereotype Maintenance". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 87 (2): 157–176. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.2.157. ISSN 1939-1315. PMID 15301625.
  10. ^ Cuadrado, Isabel; García-Ael, Cristina; Molero, Fernando (April 2015). "Gender-typing of leadership: Evaluations of real and ideal managers". Scandinavian Journal of Psychology. 56 (2): 236–244. doi:10.1111/sjop.12187. ISSN 0036-5564. PMID 25558896.
  11. ^ Clark, Travis. "A Marvel comic writer apologized after a Miles Morales story received backlash over racial stereotypes". Business Insider. Retrieved 2024-11-29.
  12. ^ Rangel, Felipe (2023-06-08). ""Didn't Realize Y'All Were Haters": Miles Morales Actor Claps Back At Backlash To MCU/Spider-Verse Comment". ScreenRant. Retrieved 2024-11-29.
  13. ^ Ma, Alexandra. "South Korea's 'Parasite' made Oscars history, but it still couldn't escape racist criticism". Business Insider. Retrieved 2024-11-29.
  14. ^ Shoichet, Catherine E. (2023-03-07). "What 'Everything Everywhere' has taught us about racism and representation in Hollywood". CNN. Retrieved 2024-11-29.
  15. ^ "Black Doctor Who fans on defying racist trolls and Ncuti Gatwa's debut". Metro. 2024-05-11. Retrieved 2024-11-29.
  16. ^ The All-White World of Children's Books, Nancy Larrick, Saturday Review, September 11, 1965, pp. 63 ff.
  17. ^ Madison Avenue versus The Feminine Mystique: How the Advertising Industry Responded to the Onset of the Modern Women's Movement, Steve Craig, presented at the Popular Culture Association conference, San Antonio, Texas, March 27, 1997.
  18. ^ Hughes, Andrew S. "'Legally Blonde' flips stereotypes". South Bend Tribune. Retrieved 2024-11-30.