Jump to content

Taxis: Difference between revisions

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia
Content deleted Content added
rv test edit / vandalism
No edit summary
 
(127 intermediate revisions by 87 users not shown)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{short description|Directed movement of a motile cell or organism in response to an external stimulus}}
{{about|the behavioural response|the vehicle|Taxicab|aircraft movement|Taxiing|the princely house|Thurn und Taxis}}
{{about|the behavioural response|the vehicle|| |Thurn und Taxis}}
{{Use dmy dates|date=April 2022}}


A '''taxis''' (plural '''taxes'''<ref>http://www.thefreedictionary.com/taxis</ref><ref>http://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/taxis</ref><ref>http://dictionary.reference.com/browse/taxis</ref> {{IPAc-en|ˈ|t|æ|k|s|iː|z}}, from the [[Ancient Greek]] {{lang|grc|[[wiktionary:τάξις|τάξις]]}}, meaning "arrangement"<ref>[http://www.perseus.tufts.edu/hopper/text?doc=Perseus:text:1999.04.0057:entry=ta/cis τάξις] in [[A Greek–English Lexicon]] by Liddell & Scott, Clarendon Press, Oxford, 1940</ref>) is the [[Motility|movement]] of an [[organism]] in response to a [[Stimulus (physiology)|stimulus]] such as light or the presence of food. Taxes are [[innate]] [[behavior]]al responses. A taxis differs from a [[tropism]] (turning response, often growth towards or away from a stimulus) in that the organism has [[motility]] and demonstrates guided movement towards or away from the stimulus source.<ref>{{cite book |author=Kendeigh, S. C. |year=1961 |title=Animal Ecology |publisher=Prentice-Hall, Inc., Englewood Cliffs, N.J. |pages=468 pp}}</ref><ref>Dusenbery, David B. (2009). ''Living at Micro Scale'', Ch. 14. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass. ISBN 978-0-674-03116-6.</ref> It is sometimes distinguished from a [[Kinesis (biology)|kinesis]], a non-directional change in activity in response to a stimulus.
A '''taxis''' ({{|grc|[[:τάξις|τάξις]]}}<ref>://www.perseus.tufts.edu/hopper/text?doc=Perseus:text:1999.04.0057:entry=ta/cis τάξις [[A Greek–English Lexicon]] Liddell Scott Clarendon Press Oxford 1940</ref>) is the [[|movement]] of an [[organism]] in response to a [[Stimulus (physiology)|stimulus]] such as light or the presence of food. Taxes are [[innate ]]al responses. A taxis differs from a [[tropism]] (turning response, often growth towards or away from a stimulus) in that the organism has [[motility]] and demonstrates guided movement towards or away from the stimulus source.<ref>{{cite book |author=Kendeigh, S. C. |year=1961 |title=Animal Ecology |publisher=Prentice-Hall, Inc., Englewood Cliffs, N.J. |pages=468 pp}}</ref><ref>Dusenbery, David B. (2009). ''Living at Micro Scale'', Ch. 14. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, ISBN978-0-674-03116-6.</ref> It is sometimes distinguished from a [[Kinesis (biology)|kinesis]], a non-directional change in activity in response to a stimulus.


== Classification ==
== Classification ==
Taxes are classified based on the type of stimulus, and on whether the organism's response is to move towards or away from the stimulus. If the moves towards the stimulus the taxis positive, while if it moves away the taxis negative. For example, flagellate [[protozoa]]ns of the genus ''[[Euglena]]'' move towards a light source. This reaction or is called positive phototaxis since phototaxis refers to a response to light and the organism is moving towards the stimulus.


== Terminology derived from ==
Taxes are classified based on the type of stimulus, and on whether the organism's response is to move towards or away from the stimulus. If the organisms moves towards the stimulus, the taxis is positive, while if it moves away, then the taxis is negative. For example, flagellate [[protozoa]]ns of the genus ''[[Euglena]]'' move towards a light source. This reaction or behaviour is called "positive phototaxis", since phototaxis refers to a response to light, and the organism is moving towards the stimulus.
Many types of taxis have been identified, including:
* ''[[Aerotaxis]]'' (stimulation by [[oxygen]])
* ''Anemotaxis'' (by [[wind]])<ref name="Mackenzie"/>
* ''Barotaxis'' (by [[pressure]])
* ''[[Chemotaxis]]'' or "gradient search" (by [[chemical]]s)<ref name="Mackenzie">{{cite journal |last1=Mackenzie |first1=Dana |title=How animals follow their nose |journal=Knowable Magazine |publisher=Annual Reviews |date=6 March 2023 |doi=10.1146/knowable-030623-4 |doi-access=free |url=https://knowablemagazine.org/article/living-world/2023/how-animals-follow-their-nose |access-date=13 March 2023 }}</ref>
* ''[[Durotaxis]]'' (by [[stiffness]])
* ''[[Electrotaxis]]'' or ''galvanotaxis'' (by [[electric current]])
* ''[[Gravitaxis]]'' or ''geotaxis'' (by [[gravity]])
* ''Hydrotaxis'' (by [[moisture]])
* ''[[Magnetotaxis]]'' (by [[magnetic field]])
* ''[[Phototaxis]]'' (by [[light]])
* ''[[Rheotaxis]]'' (by [[fluid flow]])
* ''[[Thermotaxis]]'' (by changes in [[temperature]])
* ''[[Thigmotaxis]]'' (by [[Somatosensory system|physical contact]])


Depending on the type of [[sensory organ]]s present, a taxis can be classified as a ''klinotaxis'', where an organism continuously samples the environment to determine the direction of a stimulus; a ''tropotaxis'', where bilateral sense organs are used to determine the stimulus direction; and ''telotaxis'', where a single organ suffices to establish the orientation .
Many types of taxis have been identified, including '''[[#Aerotaxis|aerotaxis]]''' (stimulation by [[oxygen]]) '''anemotaxis''' (by [[wind]]), '''barotaxis''' (by [[pressure]]), '''[[#Chemotaxis|chemotaxis]]''' (by [[chemical]]s), '''[[durotaxis]]''' (by [[stiffness]]), [[#Electrotaxis / Galvanotaxis|'''electrotaxis''' or '''galvanotaxis''']] (by [[electric current]]), '''gravitaxis''' ([[gravity]]), '''hydrotaxis''' (by [[moisture]]), '''[[#Magnetotaxis|magnetotaxis]]''' (by [[magnetic field]]), '''[[#Phototaxis|phototaxis]]''' (by [[light]]), '''[[#Rheotaxis|rheotaxis]]''' (by [[fluid flow]]), '''[[#Thermotaxis|thermotaxis]]''' (by changes in [[temperature]]) and '''thigmotaxis''' (by [[Somatosensory system|physical contact]]).


=== Terminology derived from taxis direction ===
Depending on the type of [[sensory organ]]s present, a taxis can be classified as a '''klinotaxis''', where an organism continuously samples the environment to determine the direction of a stimulus; a '''tropotaxis''', where bilateral sense organs are used to determine the stimulus direction; and '''telotaxis''', where a single organ suffices to establish the [?orientation movement].
There are five types of taxes based on the movement of organisms.

*Klinotaxis in organisms with [[receptor cell]]s but paired receptor organs. The cells for reception located all over the body, towards the anterior side. The the [[Stimulus (physiology)|stimuli]] by turning head sideways and the intensity. When the intensity of stimuli is balanced equally from all sides the in a straight line. The movement of [[]] [[]] [[]] clearly klinotaxis.
* ''Tropotaxis'' is displayed by [[organism]]s with paired receptor cells, comparing the strength of the signals and turning toward the strongest signal.<ref name="Mackenzie"/> The movement of [[Grayling (butterfly)|grayling butterflies]] and [[fish louse|fish lice]] clearly demonstrates tropotaxis.
*Telotaxis requires paired receptors. The movement occurs along the direction where the intensity of the stimuli is stronger. seen in the movement of [[bee]]s when they their [[Beehive|hive]] to look for food. They balance the stimuli from the sun as well as from [[flower]]s but on the flower whose is for them.
*Menotaxis describes organisms' maintenance of a constant [[Orientation (geometry)|angular orientation]]. A clear demonstration is shown by bees returning to their hive at night and the movement of ants with respect to the sun.
* ''Mnemotaxis'' is the use of [[memory]] to follow trails that organisms have left when travelling to or from their home.


== Examples ==
== Examples ==
<!--Alphabetical:-->
<!--Alphabetical:-->


*Aerotaxis is the response of an organism to variation in oxygen concentration, and is mainly found in aerobic bacteria.<ref name=Martin1983>{{Cite book|editor=Martin, E.A.|year=1983|title=Macmillan Dictionary of Life Sciences| edition=2nd| page=362 |place=London |publisher=Macmillan Press|isbn=0-333-34867-2|postscript=<!--None-->}}</ref>
*Aerotaxis is the response of an organism to variation in oxygen concentration, and is mainly found in aerobic bacteria.<ref name=Martin1983>{{Cite book|editor=Martin, E.A.|year=1983|title=Macmillan Dictionary of Life Sciences| edition=2nd| page=362 |place=London |publisher=Macmillan Press|isbn=0-333-34867-2}}</ref>
* Anemotaxis is the response of an organism to wind. Many insects show a positive anemotactic response (turning/flying into the wind) upon exposure to an airborne stimulus cue from a food source or pheromones.<ref name="Mackenzie"/> Cross-wind anemotactic search is exhibited by some olfactory animals in the absence of a target odor including moths, albatrosses, and polar bears.<ref>{{cite journal |last1=Kennedy |first1=J. S. |last2=Marsh |first2=D. |title=Pheromone-regulated anemotaxis in flying moths |journal=Science |date=1974 |volume=184 |issue=4140 |pages=999–1001 |doi=10.1126/science.184.4140.999|pmid=4826172 |bibcode=1974Sci...184..999K |s2cid=41768056 }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Nevitt |first1=Gabrielle A. |last2=Losekoot |first2=Marcel |last3=WeimerskirchWeimerskirch |first3=Henri |title=Evidence for olfactory search in wandering albatross, Diomedea exulans |journal=PNAS |date=2008 |volume=105 |issue=12 |pages=4576–4581|pmc=2290754 |doi=10.1073/pnas.0709047105|pmid=18326025 |doi-access=free }}</ref><ref>{{cite journal |last1=Togunov |first1=Ron |title=Windscapes and olfactory foraging in a large carnivore |journal=Scientific Reports |date=2017 |volume=7 |page=46332 |doi=10.1038/srep46332|pmid=28402340 |pmc=5389353 |bibcode=2017NatSR...746332T }}</ref> Rats have specialized supra-orbital whiskers that detect wind and cause anemotactic turning.<ref name="Mugnaini Mehrotra Davoine Sharma 2023 p. e3002168">{{cite journal |last=Mugnaini |first=Matias |last2=Mehrotra |first2=Dhruv |last3=Davoine |first3=Federico |last4=Sharma |first4=Varun |last5=Mendes |first5=Ana Rita |last6=Gerhardt |first6=Ben |last7=Concha-Miranda |first7=Miguel |last8=Brecht |first8=Michael |last9=Clemens |first9=Ann M. |date=2023 |title=Supra-orbital whiskers act as wind-sensing antennae in rats |journal=PLOS Biology |volume=21 |issue=7 |page=e3002168 |doi=10.1371/journal.pbio.3002168 |issn=1545-7885 |pmc=10325054 |pmid=37410722 |doi-access=free}}</ref>
*Anemotaxis is the response of an organism to wind. Many insects show a positive anemotactic response (turning/flying into the wind) upon exposure to an airborne stimulus cue from a food source.
*[[Chemotaxis]] is a response elicited by chemicals: that is, a response to a chemical concentration gradient.<ref name=Martin1983/> For example, chemotaxis in response to a sugar gradient has been observed in motile bacteria such as [[Escherichia coli|E. Coli]].<ref name=Blass1987>{{Cite book|author=Blass, E.M|year=1987|editor=Dobbing, J|chapter=Opioids, sweets and a mechanism for positive affect: Broad motivational implications|title=Sweetness|pages=115–124|place= London |publisher= Springer-Verlag|isbn=0-387-17045-6|postscript=<!--None-->}}</ref> Chemotaxis also occurs in the [[Antheridium|antherozoids]] of [[Marchantiophyta|liverworts]], [[fern]]s, and [[mosses]] in response to chemicals secreted by the [[Archegonium|archegonia]].<ref name=Martin1983/> Unicellular (e.g. protozoa) or multicellular (e.g. worms) organisms are targets of chemotactic substances. A concentration gradient of chemicals developed in a fluid phase guides the vectorial movement of responder cells or organisms. Inducers of locomotion towards increasing steps of concentrations are considered as [[chemoattractant]]s, while [[chemorepellent]]s result moving off the chemical. Chemotaxis is described in [[Prokaryote|prokaryotic]] and [[Eukaryote|eukaryotic]] cells, but signalling mechanisms (receptors, intracellular signaling) and effectors are significantly different.
*[[Chemotaxis]] is a response elicited by chemicals: that is, a response to a chemical concentration gradient.<ref name=Martin1983/> For example, chemotaxis in response to a sugar gradient has been observed in motile bacteria such as [[Escherichia coli|E. ]].<ref name=Blass1987>{{Cite book|author=Blass, E.M|year=1987|editor=Dobbing, J|chapter=Opioids, sweets and a mechanism for positive affect: Broad motivational implications|title=Sweetness|pages=115–124|place= London |publisher= Springer-Verlag|isbn=0-387-17045-6}}</ref> Chemotaxis also occurs in the [[Antheridium|antherozoids]] of [[Marchantiophyta|liverworts]], [[fern]]s, and [[mosses]] in response to chemicals secreted by the [[Archegonium|archegonia]].<ref name=Martin1983/> Unicellular (e.g. protozoa) or multicellular (e.g. worms) organisms are targets of chemotactic substances. A concentration gradient of chemicals developed in a fluid phase guides the vectorial movement of responder cells or organisms. Inducers of locomotion towards increasing steps of concentrations are considered as [[chemoattractant]]s, while [[chemorepellent]]s result moving off the chemical. Chemotaxis is described in [[Prokaryote|prokaryotic]] and [[Eukaryote|eukaryotic]] cells, but signalling mechanisms (receptors, intracellular signaling) and effectors are significantly different.
*[[Durotaxis]] is the directional movement of a cell along a stiffness gradient.
*[[Durotaxis]] is the directional movement of a cell along a stiffness gradient.
*Electrotaxis (or galvanotaxis) is the directional movement of motile cells in response to an [[electric field]]. It has been suggested that by detecting and orienting themselves toward the electric fields, cells are able to direct their movement towards the damages or wounds to repair the defect. It also is suggested that such a movement may contribute to directional growth of cells and tissues during development and regeneration. This notion is based on 1) the existence of measurable electric fields that naturally occur during wound healing, development and regeneration; and 2) cells in cultures respond to applied electric fields by directional cell migration – electrotaxis / galvanotaxis.
*Electrotaxis (or galvanotaxis) is the directional movement of motile cells an [[electric field]]. It has been suggested that by detecting and orienting themselves toward the electric fields, cells towards damages or wounds to repair . It also is suggested that such a movement may contribute to directional growth of cells and tissues during development and regeneration. This notion is based on the existence of measurable electric fields that naturally occur during wound healing, development and regeneration; and cells in cultures respond to applied electric fields by directional cell migration – electrotaxis/ galvanotaxis.
*Energy taxis is the orientation of bacteria towards conditions of optimal metabolic activity by sensing the internal energetic conditions of cell. Therefore in contrast to chemotaxis (taxis towards or away from a specific extracellular compound), energy taxis responds on an intracellular stimulus (e.g. [[proton motive force]], activity of [[NADH dehydrogenase|NDH- 1]]) and requires metabolic activity.<ref>Schweinitzer T, Josenhans C. [http://www.springerlink.com/content/72p224803g418g72/fulltext.pdf Bacterial energy taxis: a global strategy?] Arch Microbiol. 2010 Jul;192(7):507-20.</ref>
*Energy taxis is the orientation of bacteria towards conditions of optimal metabolic activity by sensing the internal energetic conditions of cell. Therefore in contrast to chemotaxis (taxis towards or away from a specific extracellular compound), energy taxis responds on an intracellular stimulus (e.g. [[proton motive force]], activity of [[NADH dehydrogenase|NDH- 1]]) and requires metabolic activity.<ref>Schweinitzer T, Josenhans C. [://./. Bacterial energy taxis: a global strategy?] Arch Microbiol. 2010 Jul;192(7):507-20.</ref>
*Gravitaxis (known historically as geotaxis) is a response to the attraction due to [[gravity]]. The [[plankton]]ic larvae of the [[king crab]] ''[[Lithodes aequispinus]]'' use a combination of positive phototaxis (movement towards the light) and negative gravitaxis (upward movement)&nbsp;.<ref>{{cite journal |title=Phototaxis and geotaxis of light-adapted zoeae of the golden king crab ''Lithodes aequispinus'' (Anomura: Lithodidae) in the laboratory |author=C. F. Adams & A. J. Paul |jstor=1549552 |year=1999 |journal=[[Journal of Crustacean Biology]] |volume=19 |issue=1 |pages=106–110 |doi=10.2307/1549552}}</ref> Both positive and negative gravitaxes are found in a variety of [[protozoa]]ns&nbsp;.<ref>{{cite journal |url=http://jeb.biologists.org/cgi/reprint/110/1/17 |author=T. Fenchel & B. J. Finlay |title=Geotaxis in the ciliated protozoon ''Loxodes'' |journal=Journal of Experimental Biology |date=1 May 1984|volume=110 |pages=110–133 |issue=1 }}</ref>
*Gravitaxis (known historically as geotaxis) is the to [[gravity]]. The [[plankton]]ic larvae of [[king crab]] ''[[Lithodes aequispinus]]'' positive phototaxis (movement towards the light) and negative gravitaxis (upward movement).<ref>{{cite journal |title=Phototaxis and geotaxis of light-adapted zoeae of the golden king crab ''Lithodes aequispinus'' (Anomura: Lithodidae) in the laboratory |author=C. F. Adams & A. J. Paul |jstor=1549552 |year=1999 |journal=[[Journal of Crustacean Biology]] |volume=19 |issue=1 |pages=106–110 |doi=10.2307/1549552}}</ref> Both positive and negative gravitaxes are found in a variety of [[protozoa]]ns.<ref>{{cite journal |url=http://jeb.biologists.org/cgi/reprint/110/1/17 |author=T. Fenchel & B. J. Finlay |title=Geotaxis in the ciliated protozoon ''Loxodes'' |journal=Journal of Experimental Biology |date=1 May 1984|volume=110 |pages=110–133 |issue=1 }}</ref>
*Strictly speaking, [[magnetotaxis]] is the ability to sense a [[magnetic field]] and coordinate movement in response. However, the term is commonly applied to bacteria that contain magnets and are physically rotated by the force of the [[Earth's magnetic field]]. In this case, the "behavior" has nothing to do with sensation and the bacteria are more accurately described as "magnetic bacteria".<ref>Dusenbery, David B. (2009). ''Living at Micro Scale'', pp.164-167. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Mass. ISBN 978-0-674-03116-6.</ref>
* [[]] is the ability to sense a [[magnetic field]] and coordinate movement in response. However, the term is commonly applied to bacteria that contain magnets and are physically rotated by the force of [[Earth's magnetic field]]. In this case, the "" has nothing to do with sensation and the bacteria are more accurately described as "magnetic bacteria".<ref>Dusenbery, David B. (2009). ''Living at Micro Scale'', pp.. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, ISBN978-0-674-03116-6.</ref>
* Pharotaxis is the movement to a specific location in response to learned or conditioned stimuli, or navigation by means of landmarks.<ref>{{citation| title=pharotaxis at Word Info | url=http://wordinfo.info/results/pharotaxis}}</ref><ref>{{Cite book|author=Barrows, Edward M. |year=2011 |publisher=CRC Press|edition=3, illustrated, revised|isbn=978-1-4398-3652-1|title=Animal Behavior Desk Reference: A Dictionary of Animal Behavior, Ecology, and Evolution, Third Edition|page=463|url=https://books.google.com/books?id=yaXMBQAAQBAJ&q=pharotaxis&pg=PA463}}</ref>
*Phonotaxis is the movement of an organism in response to [[sound]].
*Phonotaxis is the movement of an organism in response to [[sound]].
*[[Phototaxis]] is the movement of an organism in response to [[light]]: that is, the response to variation in light intensity and direction.<ref name=Martin1983/><ref name=Menzel1979>{{Cite book|author=Menzel, Randolf|year=1979|editor=H. Autrum (editor)|chapter=Spectral Sensitivity and Color Vision in Invertebrates |title=Comparative Physiology and Evolution of Vision in Invertebrates- A: Invertebrate Photoreceptors|location=New York|series=Handbook of Sensory Physiology|volume=VII/6A|pages=503–580. See section D: Wavelength-Specific Behavior and Color Vision|publisher=Springer-Verlag|isbn=3-540-08837-7|postscript=<!--None-->}}</ref> Negative phototaxis, or movement away from a light source, is demonstrated in some insects, such as cockroaches.<ref name=Martin1983/> Positive phototaxis, or movement towards a light source, is advantageous for phototrophic organisms as they can orient themselves most efficiently to receive light for [[photosynthesis]]. Many [[phytoflagellate]]s, e.g. ''[[Euglena]]'', and the [[chloroplast]]s of higher plants positively phototactic, moving towards a light source.<ref name=Martin1983/> Two types of positive phototaxis are observed in prokaryotes: scotophototaxis is observable as the movement of a bacterium out of the area illuminated by a microscope, when entering darkness signals the cell to reverse direction and reenter the light; a second type of positive phototaxis is true phototaxis, which is a directed movement up a gradient to an increasing amount of light.
*[[Phototaxis]] is the movement of an organism in response to [[light]]: that is, the response to variation in light intensity and direction.<ref name=Martin1983/><ref name=Menzel1979>{{Cite book|author=Menzel, Randolf|year=1979|editor=H. Autrum|chapter=Spectral Sensitivity and Color Vision in Invertebrates |title=Comparative Physiology and Evolution of Vision in Invertebrates- A: Invertebrate Photoreceptors|location=New York|series=Handbook of Sensory Physiology|volume=VII/6A|pages=503–580. See section D: Behavior and Color Vision|publisher=Springer-Verlag|isbn=3-540-08837-7}}</ref> Negative phototaxis, or movement away from a light source, is demonstrated in some insects, such as cockroaches.<ref name=Martin1983/> Positive phototaxis, or movement towards a light source, is advantageous for phototrophic organisms as they can orient themselves most efficiently to receive light for [[photosynthesis]]. Many [[phytoflagellate]]s, e.g. ''[[Euglena]]'', and the [[chloroplast]]s of higher plants positively phototactic, moving towards a light source.<ref name=Martin1983/> Two types of positive phototaxis are observed in prokaryotes: is observable as the movement of a bacterium out of the area illuminated by a microscope, when entering darkness signals the cell to reverse direction and reenter the light; a second type of positive phototaxis is true phototaxis, which is a directed movement up a gradient to an increasing amount of light.
*[[Rheotaxis]] is a response to a [[Fluid dynamics|current]] in a fluid. Positive rheotaxis is shown by fish turning to face against the current. In a flowing stream, this behavior leads them to hold their position in a stream rather than being swept downstream. Some fish will exhibit negative rheotaxis where they will avoid currents.
*[[Thermotaxis]] is a migration along a gradient of temperature. Some [[slime mold]]s and small [[nematode]]s can migrate along amazingly small temperature gradients of less than 0.1C/cm.<ref>Dusenbery, David B. (1992). ''Sensory Ecology'', p.114. W.H. Freeman, New York. ISBN 0-7167-2333-6.</ref> They apparently use this behavior to move to an optimal level in soil.<ref>Dusenbery, D.B. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 22:219-223 (1988). ''Avoided temperature leads to the surface:…''</ref><ref>Dusenbery, D.B. Biological Cybernetics, 60:431-437 (1989). ''A simple animal can use a complex stimulus patter to find a location:…''</ref>
*
[[Thermotaxis]] is a migration along a gradient of temperature. Some [[slime mold]]s and small [[nematode]]s can migrate along amazingly small temperature gradients of less than 0./cm.<ref>Dusenbery, David B. (1992). ''Sensory Ecology'', p.114. W.H. Freeman, New York. ISBN0-7167-2333-6.</ref> They apparently use this to move to an optimal level in soil.<ref>Dusenbery, D.B. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 22: (1988). Avoided temperature leads to the surface:…</ref><ref>Dusenbery, D.B. Biological Cybernetics, 60: (1989). A simple animal can use a complex stimulus patter to find a location:…</ref>
*Thigmotaxis is the response of an organism to physical contact or to the proximity of a physical discontinuity in the environment (e.g. rats preferring to swim near the edge of a water maze).
*Thigmotaxis is the response of an organism to physical contact or to the proximity of a physical discontinuity in the environment (e.g. rats preferring to swim near the edge of a water maze).

== Terminology derived from taxis direction ==
There are five types of taxes based on the movement of organisms.

*Klinotaxis occur in organisms with [[receptor cell]]s but no paired receptor organs. The cells for reception are located all over the body, particularly towards the anterior side. The organisms detect the [[Stimulus (physiology)|stimuli]] by turning their head sideways and compare the intensity. When the intensity of stimuli is balanced equally from all sides then the organisms move in a straight line. The movement of [[larva]] of [[Calliphoridae|blowfly]] and [[butterfly]] clearly demonstrate klinotaxis.
*Tropotaxis is displayed by [[organism]]s with paired receptor cells. When the stimuli coming from a source is balanced equally the organisms show movement. Because of this, animals are capable of showing sideways movement unlike klinotaxis where the organisms show movement in a straight line. The movement of [[Grayling (butterfly)|Greyling butterfly]] and [[fish louse]] clearly demonstrate tropotaxis.
*Telotaxis requires paired receptors. The movement occurs along the direction where the intensity of the stimuli is stronger. A clear demonstration of telotaxis is seen in the movement of [[bee]]s when they move from their [[Beehive|hive]] to look for food. They balance the stimuli from the sun as well as from [[flower]]s but reside on the flower whose intensity is higher for them.
*Menotaxis describes organisms' maintenance of a constant [[Orientation (geometry)|angular orientation]]. A clear demonstration is shown by bees returning to their hive at night and the movement of ants with respect to the sun.
*Mnemotaxis is a [[memory]] response of organisms to respond to their own trail. Organisms pick up the trails that they have left when traveling back to their home.


== See also ==
== See also ==
;Biology
*[[Animal locomotion]]
*[[Haptotaxis]]
*[[]]
*[[Mechanotaxis]]
*[[]]
*[[Optomotor response]]
*[[]]
*[[Tropism]]
*[[]]
* [[Tropism]]
;Different, wider context
* [[Taxonomy]], science of categorisation or classification


== References ==
== References ==
{{reflist|33em}}
{{reflist}}


== External links ==
== External links ==
* {{Cite journal |last=Nossal |first=Ralph |date=1980 |editor-last=Jäger |editor-first=Willi |editor2-last=Rost |editor2-first=Hermann |editor3-last=Tautu |editor3-first=Petre |title=Mathematical Theories of Topotaxis |url=https://link.springer.com/chapter/10.1007/978-3-642-61850-5_37 |journal=Biological Growth and Spread |location=Berlin, Heidelberg |publisher=Springer |pages=410–439 |doi=10.1007/978-3-642-61850-5_37 |isbn=978-3-642-61850-5}}
* [http://www.olfacts.nl/repL/JIP88.pdf Host-plant finding by insects: orientation, sensory input and search patterns]
* {{Cite journal |last=Vergassola |first=Massimo |last2=Villermaux |first2=Emmanuel |last3=Shraiman |first3=Boris I. |date=January 2007 |title=‘Infotaxis’ as a strategy for searching without gradients |url=https://www.nature.com/articles/nature05464 |journal=Nature |volume=445 |issue=7126 |pages=406–409 |doi=10.1038/nature05464 |issn=1476-4687}}

{{Taxes (Biology)}}
{{Authority control}}


[[Category:Taxes (biology)| ]]
[[Category:Taxes (biology)| ]]

Latest revision as of 23:22, 1 February 2025

A taxis (from Ancient Greek τάξις (táxis) 'arrangement, order';[1] pl.: taxes /ˈtæksz/)[2][3][4] is the movement of an organism in response to a stimulus such as light or the presence of food. Taxes are innate behavioural responses. A taxis differs from a tropism (turning response, often growth towards or away from a stimulus) in that in the case of taxis, the organism has motility and demonstrates guided movement towards or away from the stimulus source.[5][6] It is sometimes distinguished from a kinesis, a non-directional change in activity in response to a stimulus. taxis can be positive (moving towards the stimulus) or negative (moving away from the stimulus).

Classification

[edit]

Taxes are classified based on the type of stimulus, and on whether the organism's response is to move towards or away from the stimulus. If the organism moves towards the stimulus the taxis are positive, while if it moves away the taxis are negative. For example, flagellate protozoans of the genus Euglena move towards a light source. This reaction or behavior is called positive phototaxis since phototaxis refers to a response to light and the organism is moving towards the stimulus.

Terminology derived from type of stimulus

[edit]

Many types of taxis have been identified, including:

Depending on the type of sensory organs present, a taxis can be classified as a klinotaxis, where an organism continuously samples the environment to determine the direction of a stimulus; a tropotaxis, where bilateral sense organs are used to determine the stimulus direction; and a telotaxis, where a single organ suffices to establish the orientation of the stimulus.

Terminology derived from taxis direction

[edit]

There are five types of taxes based on the movement of organisms.

  • Klinotaxis occurs in organisms with receptor cells but not paired receptor organs. The cells for reception may be located all over the body, but often towards the anterior side. The organism detects the stimuli by turning its head sideways and comparing the intensity of the stimulus. Their direction of movement is then based on the stronger stimulus, either moving toward a desirable stimulus or away from an undesired one.[7] When the intensity of stimuli is balanced equally from all sides, the organism moves in a straight line. The movement of blowfly and butterfly larvae clearly demonstrates klinotaxis.
  • Tropotaxis is displayed by organisms with paired receptor cells, comparing the strength of the signals and turning toward the strongest signal.[7] The movement of grayling butterflies and fish lice clearly demonstrates tropotaxis.
  • Telotaxis also requires paired receptors. The movement occurs along the direction where the intensity of the stimuli is stronger. Telotaxis is clearly seen in the movement of bees when they leave their hive to look for food. They balance the stimuli from the sun as well as from flowers but land on the flower whose stimulus is most intense for them.
  • Menotaxis describes organisms' maintenance of a constant angular orientation. A clear demonstration is shown by bees returning to their hive at night and the movement of ants with respect to the sun.
  • Mnemotaxis is the use of memory to follow trails that organisms have left when travelling to or from their home.

Examples

[edit]
  • Aerotaxis is the response of an organism to variation in oxygen concentration, and is mainly found in aerobic bacteria.[8]
  • Anemotaxis is the response of an organism to wind. Many insects show a positive anemotactic response (turning/flying into the wind) upon exposure to an airborne stimulus cue from a food source or pheromones.[7] Cross-wind anemotactic search is exhibited by some olfactory animals in the absence of a target odor including moths, albatrosses, and polar bears.[9][10][11] Rats have specialized supra-orbital whiskers that detect wind and cause anemotactic turning.[12]
  • Chemotaxis is a response elicited by chemicals: that is, a response to a chemical concentration gradient.[8][7][13] For example, chemotaxis in response to a sugar gradient has been observed in motile bacteria such as E. coli.[14] Chemotaxis also occurs in the antherozoids of liverworts, ferns, and mosses in response to chemicals secreted by the archegonia.[8] Unicellular (e.g. protozoa) or multicellular (e.g. worms) organisms are targets of chemotactic substances. A concentration gradient of chemicals developed in a fluid phase guides the vectorial movement of responder cells or organisms. Inducers of locomotion towards increasing steps of concentrations are considered as chemoattractants, while chemorepellents result moving off the chemical. Chemotaxis is described in prokaryotic and eukaryotic cells, but signalling mechanisms (receptors, intracellular signaling) and effectors are significantly different.
  • Durotaxis is the directional movement of a cell along a stiffness gradient.
  • Electrotaxis (or galvanotaxis) is the directional movement of motile cells along the vector of an electric field. It has been suggested that by detecting and orienting themselves toward the electric fields, cells can move towards damages or wounds to repair them. It also is suggested that such a movement may contribute to directional growth of cells and tissues during development and regeneration. This notion is based on the existence of measurable electric fields that naturally occur during wound healing, development and regeneration; and cells in cultures respond to applied electric fields by directional cell migration – electrotaxis / galvanotaxis.
  • Energy taxis is the orientation of bacteria towards conditions of optimal metabolic activity by sensing the internal energetic conditions of cell. Therefore, in contrast to chemotaxis (taxis towards or away from a specific extracellular compound), energy taxis responds on an intracellular stimulus (e.g. proton motive force, activity of NDH- 1) and requires metabolic activity.[15]
  • Gravitaxis (known historically as geotaxis) is the directional movement (along the vector of gravity) to the center of gravity. The planktonic larvae of a king crab, Lithodes aequispinus, combine positive phototaxis (movement towards the light) and negative gravitaxis (upward movement).[16] Also the larvae of a polychaete, Platynereis dumerilii, combine positive phototaxis (movement to the light coming from the water surface) and UV-induced positive gravitaxis (downward movement) to form a ratio-chromatic depth-gauge.[17] Both positive and negative gravitaxes are found in a variety of protozoans (e.g., Loxodes, Remanella and Paramecium).[18]
  • Magnetotaxis is, strictly speaking, the ability to sense a magnetic field and coordinate movement in response. However, the term is commonly applied to bacteria that contain magnets and are physically rotated by the force of Earth's magnetic field. In this case, the "behaviour" has nothing to do with sensation and the bacteria are more accurately described as "magnetic bacteria".[19]
  • Pharotaxis is the movement to a specific location in response to learned or conditioned stimuli, or navigation by means of landmarks.[20][21]
  • Phonotaxis is the movement of an organism in response to sound.
  • Phototaxis is the movement of an organism in response to light: that is, the response to variation in light intensity and direction.[8][22] Negative phototaxis, or movement away from a light source, is demonstrated in some insects, such as cockroaches.[8] Positive phototaxis, or movement towards a light source, is advantageous for phototrophic organisms as they can orient themselves most efficiently to receive light for photosynthesis. Many phytoflagellates, e.g. Euglena, and the chloroplasts of higher plants positively phototactic, moving towards a light source.[8] Two types of positive phototaxis are observed in prokaryotes: scotophobotaxis is observable as the movement of a bacterium out of the area illuminated by a microscope, when entering darkness signals the cell to reverse direction and reenter the light; a second type of positive phototaxis is true phototaxis, which is a directed movement up a gradient to an increasing amount of light. There is a different classification to orientation towards dark areas called scototaxis.
  • Rheotaxis is a response to a current in a fluid. Positive rheotaxis is shown by fish turning to face against the current. In a flowing stream, this behaviour leads them to hold their position in a stream rather than being swept downstream. Some fish will exhibit negative rheotaxis where they will avoid currents.
  • Thermotaxis is a migration along a gradient of temperature. Some slime molds and small nematodes can migrate along amazingly small temperature gradients of less than 0.1 °C/cm.[23] They apparently use this behaviour to move to an optimal level in soil.[24][25]
  • Thigmotaxis is the response of an organism to physical contact or to the proximity of a physical discontinuity in the environment (e.g. rats preferring to swim near the edge of a water maze). Codling moth larvae are believed to use thigmotactic sense to locate fruits to feed on.[26] Mice and rats, when inhabiting human-made structures, tend to stick close to vertical surfaces; this primarily manifests as running along the floor/wall juncture. Whiskers (vibrissae) are often used to detect the presence of a wall or surface in the absence of sufficient light in rodents and felines to aid in thigmotaxis.

See also

[edit]
Biology
Different, wider context
  • Taxonomy, science of categorisation or classification

References

[edit]
  1. ^ Henry George Liddell; Robert Scott (1940). "τάξις". A Greek–English Lexicon. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
  2. ^ "taxis" – via The Free Dictionary.
  3. ^ "taxis". Merriam-Webster.com Dictionary. Merriam-Webster.
  4. ^ "taxis". Dictionary.com Unabridged (Online). n.d.
  5. ^ Kendeigh, S. C. (1961). Animal Ecology. Prentice-Hall, Inc., Englewood Cliffs, N.J. pp. 468 pp.
  6. ^ Dusenbery, David B. (2009). Living at Micro Scale, Ch. 14. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts ISBN 978-0-674-03116-6.
  7. ^ a b c d e f Mackenzie, Dana (6 March 2023). "How animals follow their nose". Knowable Magazine. Annual Reviews. doi:10.1146/knowable-030623-4. Retrieved 13 March 2023.
  8. ^ a b c d e f Martin, E.A., ed. (1983). Macmillan Dictionary of Life Sciences (2nd ed.). London: Macmillan Press. p. 362. ISBN 0-333-34867-2.
  9. ^ Kennedy, J. S.; Marsh, D. (1974). "Pheromone-regulated anemotaxis in flying moths". Science. 184 (4140): 999–1001. Bibcode:1974Sci...184..999K. doi:10.1126/science.184.4140.999. PMID 4826172. S2CID 41768056.
  10. ^ Nevitt, Gabrielle A.; Losekoot, Marcel; WeimerskirchWeimerskirch, Henri (2008). "Evidence for olfactory search in wandering albatross, Diomedea exulans". PNAS. 105 (12): 4576–4581. doi:10.1073/pnas.0709047105. PMC 2290754. PMID 18326025.
  11. ^ Togunov, Ron (2017). "Windscapes and olfactory foraging in a large carnivore". Scientific Reports. 7: 46332. Bibcode:2017NatSR...746332T. doi:10.1038/srep46332. PMC 5389353. PMID 28402340.
  12. ^ Mugnaini, Matias; Mehrotra, Dhruv; Davoine, Federico; Sharma, Varun; Mendes, Ana Rita; Gerhardt, Ben; Concha-Miranda, Miguel; Brecht, Michael; Clemens, Ann M. (2023). "Supra-orbital whiskers act as wind-sensing antennae in rats". PLOS Biology. 21 (7): e3002168. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.3002168. ISSN 1545-7885. PMC 10325054. PMID 37410722.
  13. ^ Reddy, Gautam; Murthy, Venkatesh N.; Vergassola, Massimo (10 March 2022). "Olfactory Sensing and Navigation in Turbulent Environments". Annual Review of Condensed Matter Physics. 13 (1): 191–213. Bibcode:2022ARCMP..13..191R. doi:10.1146/annurev-conmatphys-031720-032754. ISSN 1947-5454. S2CID 243966350.
  14. ^ Blass, E.M (1987). "Opioids, sweets and a mechanism for positive affect: Broad motivational implications". In Dobbing, J (ed.). Sweetness. London: Springer-Verlag. pp. 115–124. ISBN 0-387-17045-6.
  15. ^ Schweinitzer T, Josenhans C. Bacterial energy taxis: a global strategy? Arch Microbiol. 2010 Jul;192(7):507-20.
  16. ^ C. F. Adams & A. J. Paul (1999). "Phototaxis and geotaxis of light-adapted zoeae of the golden king crab Lithodes aequispinus (Anomura: Lithodidae) in the laboratory". Journal of Crustacean Biology. 19 (1): 106–110. doi:10.2307/1549552. JSTOR 1549552.
  17. ^ Verasztó, Csaba; Gühmann, Martin; Jia, Huiyong; Rajan, Vinoth Babu Veedin; Bezares-Calderón, Luis A.; Piñeiro-Lopez, Cristina; Randel, Nadine; Shahidi, Réza; Michiels, Nico K.; Yokoyama, Shozo; Tessmar-Raible, Kristin; Jékely, Gáspár (29 May 2018). "Ciliary and rhabdomeric photoreceptor-cell circuits form a spectral depth gauge in marine zooplankton". eLife. 7. doi:10.7554/eLife.36440. PMC 6019069. PMID 29809157.
  18. ^ T. Fenchel & B. J. Finlay (1 May 1984). "Geotaxis in the ciliated protozoon Loxodes". Journal of Experimental Biology. 110 (1): 110–133. doi:10.1242/jeb.110.1.17.
  19. ^ Dusenbery, David B. (2009). Living at Micro Scale, pp.164–167. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, Massachusetts ISBN 978-0-674-03116-6.
  20. ^ pharotaxis at Word Info
  21. ^ Barrows, Edward M. (2011). Animal Behavior Desk Reference: A Dictionary of Animal Behavior, Ecology, and Evolution, Third Edition (3, illustrated, revised ed.). CRC Press. p. 463. ISBN 978-1-4398-3652-1.
  22. ^ Menzel, Randolf (1979). "Spectral Sensitivity and Color Vision in Invertebrates". In H. Autrum (ed.). Comparative Physiology and Evolution of Vision in Invertebrates- A: Invertebrate Photoreceptors. Handbook of Sensory Physiology. Vol. VII/6A. New York: Springer-Verlag. pp. 503–580. See section D: Wavelength–Specific Behavior and Color Vision. ISBN 3-540-08837-7.
  23. ^ Dusenbery, David B. (1992). Sensory Ecology, p.114. W.H. Freeman, New York. ISBN 0-7167-2333-6.
  24. ^ Dusenbery, D.B. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 22:219–223 (1988). "Avoided temperature leads to the surface:…"
  25. ^ Dusenbery, D.B. Biological Cybernetics, 60:431–437 (1989). "A simple animal can use a complex stimulus patter to find a location:…"
  26. ^ Jackson, D. Michael (15 May 1982). "Searching Behavior and Survival of 1st-Instar Codling Moths". Annals of the Entomological Society of America. 75 (3): 284–289. doi:10.1093/aesa/75.3.284. ISSN 0013-8746.
[edit]